Traumatic memories can severely
affect a person's quality of life when they become intrusive thoughts that lead
to anxiety and continue to cause distress. For this reason, scientists are now
looking into ways of weakening such memories and lessening their impact.
People who experience
traumatic events may find their memories haunt them for a long time after the
experience occurred.
Exposure to trauma can
trigger numerous mental and emotional problems, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
and anxietydisorders, for example, phobias.
Ways of treating people who
experience the long-term effects of trauma can include cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)
and other types of psychotherapy, as well as specific drug prescriptions to
address symptoms of depression or
anxiety.
However, increasingly,
researchers are exploring ways of acting on the traumatic memories that cause
an individual's long-term distress.
Such is the case with a team
of scientists from five research institutions across three countries:
Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Reina
Sofia–CIEN Foundation in Madrid, Spain, New York University, and the Radboud
University Medical Centre in Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
These researchers have been
looking into a novel way of weakening people's distressing memories and
reducing their psychological impact.
Can
we manipulate 'established' memories?
In a new study
paper that appears in the journal Science Advances and whose first author
is Ana Galarza Vallejo, the researchers write that "[a]n effective
treatment for these disorders [related to trauma] should selectively decrease
these intrusive, pathological memories."
At the same time, they note
that the prevailing opinion in memory research has been that "established
memories are relatively fixed" and, thus, cannot be easily modified. Yet,
in their new study, the investigators show that acting on disturbing memories
is, in fact, a clear possibility.
"[M]emories are initially labile and sensitive
to interference by, e.g., electroconvulsive therapy, general anesthesia, or
protein synthesis inhibition, but stabilize over time during a period of
consolidation, after which memories were considered to be established and no
longer sensitive to disruption or modification," the authors write.
However, the scientists
observe that past research that researchers conducted
using animal models suggested that reactivating an already established memory
can, for a short time, make it "vulnerable" to external
modifications.
Building on this existing
evidence, they decided to work with a group of human participants, and test an
intervention they based on administering the anesthetic propofol.
Well-timed
sedative acts on bad memories
In the current study,
Vallejo and colleagues recruited 50 healthy participants, in whom they first
instilled unwanted memories by asking them to watch two narrated slideshows.
Both of these slideshows featured negative emotional content about midway
through.
To reactive the bad
memories, the researchers called the participants back after 1 week, and showed
them the first slide from one of the two presentations, asking them targeted
questions.
Once the participants
started recalling the unwanted memories, the investigators sedated them with
propofol, the anesthetic whose potential in memory-manipulation the team wanted
to assess.
Then, the researchers
assigned the participants to one of two groups. Following on 24 hours after the
propofol injection, the people in the first groups had to take a test
evaluating their recall of the stories in each of the two slideshows — both the
one which they had had to remember before sedation and the one which they were
not prompted to recall.
As for the participants in
the second group, they took the same tests immediately after having received
the propofol intervention.
The investigators found that 24 hours after its
administration, propofol had effectively disrupted the reconsolidation of the
bad memory that researchers had asked participants to recall.
Thus, while the individuals
in the first group were still able to remember the negative memory they
associated with the slideshow that they had not recalled before the sedation,
their memory of the reactivated story was weaker.
No comments:
Post a Comment