If you want some
thrills this Halloween, you should try tricking yourself for a change. In this
Spotlight, we look at some spooky experiments that'll trick the brain and ask
intriguing questions about consciousness and perception.
Treat yourself to some scientific 'spooks' this
Halloween.
In Act One, Scene Five of William Shakespeare's play Hamlet, the main character, after meeting his father's
ghost, remarks to his best friend, "There are more things in heaven and
earth, Horatio,/Than are dreamt of in your philosophy."
Hamlet is referring to the world being filled with spooky mysteries that
we may have a hard time even imagining.
Perhaps one of the most mysterious things on earth is, in fact, the human
brain.
How does our consciousness work? Can we rely on our senses, or do they —
and the brain — often trick us?
In this Spotlight, we look at a series of spooky experiments that shed
some light on how our brains work, and which might make you question your own
senses.
So, if you are in the mood to test the limits of your perception this
Halloween, why not try to trick your own brain by replicating one of the
experiments below?
1. The ghost in the mirror
One legend that used to be popular among schoolchildren has it that if you
look into a mirror by the light of a candle and recite "Bloody Mary"
three times, a woman's specter will appear in the glass.
In the past, young women supposedly performed other, similar rituals in
the hope that they'd catch a glimpse of their future husbands in the dimly lit
surface of the mirror.
It turns out that while peering into a mirror in a
dimly lit room will bring about no supernatural events, it will likely reveal
to the viewer one or several strange faces — sometimes with a fearsome, and at
other times a benevolent, expression. How so?
What will you see if you peer at your own reflection
in a mirror placed in a dimly lit room?
This is what Giovanni Caputo, in the Department of Psychology at the
University of Urbino in Italy, set out to answer.
In his study, Caputo recreated a visual illusion that occurs when a person
stares at their own face in the mirror in a room with poor lighting.
The researcher used "a relatively large mirror" of 0.5 x 0.5
meters, which he placed in a room lit by "a 25-Watt incandescent
light," although he notes that to recreate this experiment, the exact same
conditions are not necessary.
Each volunteer sat 0.4 meters away from the mirror, and they had about 10
minutes to peer into it; though the illusion, Caputo says, usually manifested
within about 1 minute.
At the end of the session, the participants wrote down what they had seen
in the mirror, and their descriptions varied greatly. Out of a total of 50
participants:
·
66
percent reported seeing "huge deformations" of their own faces
·
18
percent saw "a parent's face with traits changed," with 10 percent of
these seeing the faces of deceased parents, and 8 percent those of parents who
were still alive
·
28
percent saw "an unknown person"
·
another
28 percent reported seeing "an archetypal face, such as that of an old
woman, a child, or a portrait of an ancestor"
·
18
percent saw the face of an animal
·
48
percent saw "fantastical and monstrous beings"
The
Troxler effect or spectral apparitions?
First and foremost, this visual illusion seems to appear due to the fact
that the eyes are forced to fixate on a single point. In this respect, the
faces in the mirror illusion could be compared to an optical illusion called
"Troxler's fading" or the "Troxler effect."
If you stare at the red dot in the center for long
enough, the blue circle will soon start to fade away.
Image credit: Mysid, Wikimedia Commons
Image credit: Mysid, Wikimedia Commons
This phenomenon — which Ignaz Paul Vital Troxler discovered in 1804 —
occurs when someone stares fixedly at a single point.
When it starts to happen, anything surrounding that point, particularly
splashes of color, will begin to fade away.
As a result, it might seem as though we have temporarily lost our ability
to perceive colors.
This likely happens as a result of "neural adaptation,"
in which our nerve cells ignore stimuli that are not essential to perceiving
the object of our focus.
Therefore, we end up seeing the one thing that we are fixing our gazes on
and little or nothing else. This, however, is not the case with the faces in
the mirror illusion, Caputo says.
"[This] explanation," he writes,
"would predict that face traits should fade away and eventually disappear, whereas the apparitions in the mirror
consist of new faces having new traits."
Instead, what may happen is that by staring continually at our own faces,
the stimuli initially stop connecting in a meaningful way, so that we are
unable to "string together" the facial traits we perceive.
This may result in a spontaneous reassembly of these traits, so it may
seem to us that our faces have become deformed or uncanny. However, this fails
to explain all, Caputo suggests.
"[The] frequent apparition of fantastical and monstrous beings,"
he writes, "and of animal faces cannot [...] be explained by any actual
theory of face processing."
The 'other' that we project
So, what happens? It seems likely that once our vision is disrupted, our
brains start projecting fears or desires onto the distorted traits in the
mirror, giving them new identities and purposes.
Caputo inferred this when analyzing the participants' emotional responses
to their individual mirror "apparitions." Depending on what they
thought they saw, the volunteers often felt either scared or happy.
"Some
participants saw a malign expression on the 'other' face and became anxious.
Other participants felt that the 'other' was smiling or cheerful, and
experienced positive emotions in response. The apparition of deceased parents
or of archetypal portraits produced feelings of silent query."
Giovanni Caputo
According to him, the apparition of strange faces in the mirror, to which
we then respond in such a strong emotional way, is probably due to the fact
that the complex process of self-identity construction — which we go through
each time we see our reflections — is disrupted.
This, he thinks, may cause a "potential breakdown of
self-identity" that we experience as a spooky dissociation.
2. Is that your hand?
There is little that we are as certain of as the fact that we own every
inch of our bodies. Well...this is true for most of us, at least.
The rubber hand illusion can make you think that an
artificial hand has replaced your real one.
Following serious health events such as brain lesions, a person may
experience something called "somatoparaphrenia."
This is a sense of dissociation from a part of or even the entire body.
In other words, a person will believe that a limb, some other body part,
or their entire body does not belong to them.
These may seem like extreme cases, but some simple
experiments have shown that pretty much all of us can be tricked into
dissociating from our bodies, or claiming artificial body parts or even
"ghost" limbs as our own.
The most famous experiment conducted in this sense is that of the rubber
hand. In this experiment, a dark screen shields one of the participant's arms
from their sight.
Instead, the researchers place a rubber arm in front of the participant.
Then, they repeatedly tickle both the rubber hand and the participant's hidden
real hand at the same time.
At this point, the volunteer has surprisingly taken ownership of the
rubber arm and seems to react as though their own real hand has been tickled.
In the video below, put together by National Geographic, you can see a
variation of the "rubber hand illusion" experiment:
Movement and the sense of self
In a study focused on the rubber hand illusion, a team of researchers from
the University of Milan, the University of Milan Medical School, and the
University of Turin — all in Italy — wanted to see what happens in the brain
when a person experiences this strange illusion.
The investigators found "that body ownership
and the motor system are mutually interactive and both contribute to the
dynamic construction of bodily self-awareness in healthy and pathological
brains."
In other words, MRI scans showed
that when participants began to believe that the rubber hand was their own, the
brain networks that coordinated movement in the real hand began to slow down.
"The present findings," they explain, "which shed new light
on our understanding of the different aspects that contribute to the formation
of a coherent self-awareness, suggest that bodily self-consciousness strictly
depends on the possibility of movement."
3. What the brain hears
Our sense of hearing helps us navigate the world. It is fairly easy to
trick this sense — but particular experiences can tell us a lot about how our
brains can actually control what we hear.
Do we only hear what we have already learned to hear?
Earlier this year, a cryptic audio track went
viral. The catch? People could not agree on whether the recorded voice was
saying the word "Yanny" or the word "Laurel."
Why do people hear different names, however? One explanation has to do
with pitch, or audio frequency, and how each person's ears are
"tuned."
So, some people may hear "Yanny" while others will hear
"Laurel."
However, according to Prof. Hugh McDermott — at Melbourne's Bionics
Institute in Autralia — who spoke to the newspaper The Guardian,
the story is more complex than that; it may have to do with how our brains
process information.
Because the track is auditively ambiguous, our brains have to choose their
own "interpretation" — but how do they do that?
"When the brain is uncertain of something, it uses surrounding cues
to help you make the right decision," explains Prof. Mc Dermott.
"If you
heard a conversation happening around you regarding 'Laurel' you wouldn't have
heard 'Yanny.' Personal history can also give an unconscious preference for one
or another. You could know many people named 'Laurel' and none called
'Yanny.'"
Prof. Hugh McDermott
Your brain, the anticipator
In other words, our brains are able to make sense of things by
anticipating them. That is, if we have already learned something, only then are
we able to identify it. That is what makes the difference between hearing
gibberish and hearing a sentence that makes actual sense.
This is why our brains make choices when presented
with ambiguous stimuli or information. A good example of this is sine-wave
speech, which consists of computer-altering voices so they are almost
unrecognizable.
Take these examples that
researchers at the University of Sussex in the United Kingdom created. If you
listen to this track,
it is unlikely that you will be able to make head or tail of it.
However, if you listen to the original unaltered
recording first, and then to the sine-wave track, you will have no trouble in
understanding the sentence, despite the distortion.
Perhaps the reason ghosts so easily spook us is that we do not have a
clear understanding of how our consciousness works. Some of the discoveries
surrounding the workings of our brains are, in themselves, spooky.
A 1992 survey revealed that 10–15 percent of
the respondents, based in the United States, have experienced some sort of
sensory hallucination at one point in their lives.
When our bodies and minds can be so easily tricked, there is little wonder
that the ghosts and ghouls of Halloween still hold such fascination over so
many of us.
No comments:
Post a Comment